Friday, March 29, 2019

Study into Mapping Blind Spots

Study into routine Blind SpotsThough the blind tell is a character of the retina that is devoid of photoreceptors, therefore relaying no visual information, it does not lead to the go steady of a deplorable hole in our visual field (Sakaguchi, 2001). This is ascribable to the perceptual phenomenon of filling-in, whereby a visual attribute such as annotate and brightness is perceived in the blind-spot, even though it notwithstanding exists in the surround (Komatsu, 2006).A long-standing question has been whether perceptual filling-in ignores this absence of information or actively fills it in (De Weerd, 2006). The symbolic theory proposes that too soon visual areas only extract information at the step up b nightspot with the coloration and execute of the surface reconstructed in higher areas (Komatsu, 2006). Conversely, the isomorphic theory assumes that the retinotopic symbolize in the primary visual cortex (V1) receives information from the entire surface with visual fea tures such as tinge activated in early visual areas (De Weerd, 2006). and thence Komatsu (2006) proposes an amalgamation of the theories, that neural activity is higher along the edge of the blind-spot with these signals then spreading perilwise a two-dimensional soldiery of visual feature sensitive cells in early visual areas. hence the mechanisms of filling-in depend upon activity along the physiological edge of the blind-spot as Spillmann, Ottee, hamburger and Magnussen (2006) found that a ring as thin as 0.5 had been fit in inducing people of touch in filling-in. Li et al., (2014) analysed this through 2.5 mono-coloured annuli, finding that it warp in completely, providing evidence for active colour filling-in from a gloomy border.Fahle and Schmid (1987) proposed that the loaded standoffishness between photoreceptors is slightly higher for the secular slope compared to the os bonye facial expression in the retina with the ganglion cells distributed in a simi lar irregular fashion. This spacial arrangement of the image as it is abuted within the retina is maintained in V1 (De Weerd, 2006). Hence Li et al., (2014) extended the area of homogenous stimuli to bi-coloured rings, revealing the heraldic bearing of a retinotopic rule in perceptual filling-in that favours the nasal side. Whilst this validates rapid colour filling-in as preattentive, these spatial arrangements may be affected by separate preattentive factors which contribute to global processes such as recounting strikingness (Brown Thurmond, 1993).Hence the current study aimed to examine ambiguities in perceptual filling-in when responding to both unhorse and higher-level processes. More specifically, whether differences in the intercourse strikingness of bi-coloured annuli affected the nasal or worldly affect (retinotopic rule) in filling-in blind-spot. As Brown and Thurmond (1993) infer that intercourse salience contributes to higher processes, exposure to increas ed intensiveness may alter the retinotopic rule. Based on these two studies, it was hypothesised that the average plectrum chance for round-backed filling-in would decline as the proportional saturation for the nasal side decreases. Reciprocally, it was hypothesised than lopsided choice probability would increase as telling saturation for the temporal side decreases.MethodParticipantsFifteen students from the University of Sydney (4 men, 11 women M age = 21, SD = 2.03), participated voluntarily. altogether instrumentalists had normal or corrected-to normal vision. With the exception of the cardinal experimenters, participants were nave to the experiment.MaterialsStimuli only stimuli was generated using Microsoft PowerPoint Software. Stimuli consisted of bi-coloured (red and green) annuli, with a width of 2.5 (derived from Li et al., 2014). For each participant, the diameter of the stimuli was familiarized so that the annuli oerlapped with the edge of the blind spot. The two halves of the bi-coloured annuli were juxtaposed symmetrically on the nasal and temporal sides of the blind spot. Each side was counterbalanced a busy trials wherein half(prenominal) the trials were comprised of nasal-red temporal-green stimuli and the other half, nasal-green temporal-red. The saturation was adjusted for one side to cytosine%, 50% or 25% of the original saturation, while the other half was maintained at vitamin C% saturation (that is, 100100, 10050, 10025, 50100, 25100). Controls used reversed stimuli, such that the fixation cross appeared on the right and the stimuli on the go away. Thus, 36 randomised trials were conducted, consisting of six controls and three repeats of ten test stimuli (Appendix A1).Choice Panel This illustrated the spread of the two colours in coloured record books and consisted of ten choices (refer to Appendix A2).ProcedureThe experiment was conducted over two sessions, one week apart. In the first session, the blind spot of each pa rticipant was typifyped using Microsoft PowerPoint. Participants were seated in a dark room with a chin rest at a distance of .57m away from an ASUS S400c 14-inch screen. Participants were instructed to fixate on a white fixation cross presented on a black primer with their right eye and left eye covered. Using a digital pen tool, a small white test dot was moved across the screen by the experimenter. The positions where the dot disappeared and reappeared were verbally report by the participant and digitally pronounced when it was not visible. The process was repeated until the blind-spot had been mapped out adequately.In the second session, participant were asked to report the perceptual filling-in of the blind-spot. The fixation slide (5 sec) and the stimulus slide was presented (3 sec). Participants were then presented with the choice panel and asked to report the choice that best resembled what they observed. At the goal of the study, participants verbally reported their expe riences with filling-in (Appendix B).ResultsPaired sample t-tests were conducted, with participants reporting nasal colour dominance significantly much often than either symmetrical filling-in, t(1,14) = 2.37, p =.03 (nasal red M = 40.1%) and t(1,14) = 3.09, p M = 51.2%), or temporal colour dominance t(1,14) = 5.79, p M = 60.1%) and t(1,14) = 9.13, p M = 75.6%).A 5 x 2, repeated measure analysis of variance was carried out on colour and relative saturation on average choice probability for nasal dominance filling-in, after which quadratic trend origins were conducted. Averaged over relative saturation, choice probability for nasal dominance filling-in was significantly greater when the nasal colour was green than red (mean difference = 21.4%), F(1,14) = 15.30, p F(4, 56) = 3.56, p = .01. quadratic polynomial trend contrasts revealed that this burden increased significantly as the nasal relative saturation increased from 25% to 100%, but decreased as relative saturation decreased from 100 to 25 on the temporal side, F(1,14) = 19.92, p F(1,14) = 0.00, p = .99, nor was there a significant cubic trend, F(1,14) = .37, p = .55.Figure 1. Average choice probability for nasal dominance filling-in as function of type of relative saturation (N = 15).DiscussionParticipants reported asymmetrical nasal dominance filling-in significantly more than symmetrical or temporally dominant filling-in. This replicated preliminary findings by Li et al., (2014), that filling-in from the nasal side set-aside(p) a much larger region than filling-in from the temporal side.A colour effect was revealed with a preference for green over red whilst authoritative for salience, which Hamburger, Prior, Sarris and Spillmann, (2005) account for through higher-level processing of colour information. They postulate that typical background colours in natural scenes, i.e. green and blue, fill-in more tardily than colours that are attributed to the foreground i.e. red and yellow. Hence in afterli fe studies, more colour pairings including blue and yellow should be tested to validate this theory. Yet, this colour effect may also reveal that relative salience was poorly controlled for in this study, as Brown and Thurmond (1993) manipulated saturation, reporting that a more salient colour is favoured when filling-in. This is because the green provided more contrast than the red as Hamburger et al., (2005) suggests that perceptual qualities of surfaces, e.g., saturation, affect other properties such as contrast and luminance. This raises concerns in the current study in the measure of relative salience, as is it unclear whether exchanges in saturation alone prompted a global process that overruled the local processes involved in filling-in. Hence, in future studies these visual characteristics choose to be carefully controlled for to ensure that it does not have a self-contradictory effect on salience.As predicted, asymmetrical choice probability declined as the relative satu ration for the nasal side decreased. This concurs with Li et al., (2014) that the strength of colour filling-in is determined by the retinotopic rule, whereby the direction of filling-in is correlated to greater cortical bulge on the nasal side. Furthermore, Fahle and Schmid (1987) contend that the nasal side has a disdain contrast sensitivity threshold compared to the temporal side which implies that the receptors on the nasal side were more easily able to detect a change in saturation, which increased the relative salience of the temporal side. This offered access to greater filling-in, which decreased the nasal-preference for asymmetrical filling-in of the disk. This can be extended for future look into by also examining the effect of an increase in relative saturation e.g., 150%, 200%, 300% which may have an additive effect by strengthening the retinotopic rule (Brown Thurmond, 1993).However, contrary to the hypothesis that the asymmetrical choice probability would increase as relative saturation for the temporal side decreased, nasal dominant filling-in decreased as saturation decreased on the temporal side. As mentioned, the perceptual qualities of surfaces interact as Komatsu (2006) implies that the brain needs to integrate lower level visual information such as colour and brightness and decode it at the retinotopic map. This signal is then patrimonial to higher cortical areas to eventually generate surface lore. Cortical processing in these early visual systems are heavily biased toward the staining of local contrast in luminance, resulting from edges, which is necessary in surface perception (De Weerd, 2006). In the present study when saturation was decreased on the temporal side, it also changed the luminance of the green colour, making it more salient relative to the red, amplifying the edge between the two colours. Hence, considering this local processing preference for variations in luminance, Sakaguchi (2001) contended that the physical e dge that exists in the annulus can activate the neurons code them, as the two colours differ in luminance. This increased the salience of the temporal side, permitting a greater percentage of the temporal side of the disk to fill-in.Another limitation of the present study is that a majority of participants verbally reported seeing a black spot mainly on the temporal side, implying that the annulus did not fill-in completely. Yet as Li et al., (2014) argues that colour perception processes are rapid and preattentive, this incomplete filling-in cannot be attributed to an curt fixation time. Rather, this can be explained by other methodological issues as Spillmann et al., (2006) attribute this partial filling-in to improper fixation and involuntary eye movements that burn down the annulus relative to the blind-spot. Spillmann et al., (2006) highlight the significance of this partial-filling in effect in validating that filling-in is an active physiological process generated by a sh orten edge at the blind-spot. However as this dark shadow was reported mainly on the temporal side it can be explained by the more sparse distribution of receptors on the retinotopic map resulting in weaker temporal processes. As the width of the annuli remained constant, the nasal side filled in break in due to a denser distribution of receptors (Li et al., 2014). Hence future studies should consider the relative width of the stimulus to suggest a width for the temporal side of the annulus in order to achieve symmetrical filling-in.In summation, this study presents evidence for active neural processes in retinotopically organized lower order areas, but also a role for higher order cognitive factors such as surface description (De Weerd, 2006). In the future, studies should attempt to map the size of the activated brain area to endorse this retinotopic asymmetry during filling-in and the do of relative salience on this symmetry (Li et al., 2014).

No comments:

Post a Comment

Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.